Alexitch, Louise
Borowsky, Ron
Borowsky, R., & Besner, D. (1993). Visual word recognition: A multistage activation model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(4), 813. | These experiments and model demonstrated the advantages of a stages-of-processing framework over parallel distributed processing for understanding lexical reading processes (examining semantic priming, stimulus quality, and word frequency), and also led to an interesting exchange with Plaut & Booth in Psychological Review over a decade later (Borowsky & Besner, 2006; Besner & Borowsky, 2006). Perhaps most interesting was that they declined the opportunity for a final rebuttal... |
Borowsky, R., & Masson, M. E. (1996). Semantic ambiguity effects in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(1), 63. | These experiments and neural network simulations were the first to demonstrate how different characteristics of neural networks can simultaneously account for: (1) semantic ambiguity advantages (e.g., in a lexical decision task you identify ambiguous words like 'bat' sooner than unambiguous words like 'bet', which is simulated in the network by a measure of familiarity in the semantic system), (2) disadvantages (e.g., eye-tracking gaze duration is longer for ambiguous words when reading for meaning, which is simulated in the network by the time for the activation pattern to settle in the semantic system), and null effects (no differences in naming reaction time, as phonological processing can proceed without influence from semantics). |
Borowsky, R., Cummine, J., Owen, W. J., Friesen, C. K., Shih, F., & Sarty, G. E. (2006). FMRI of ventral and dorsal processing streams in basic reading processes: insular sensitivity to phonology. Brain topography, 18(4), 233-239. | This fMRI study was the first to compare reading aloud of exception words (e.g., ‘one’, which must be read via lexical memory) and pseudohomophones (e.g., ‘wun’, which must be read via sublexical spelling to sound translation) to examine the cortical visual processing streams as well as the insular cortex, and their relationship to lexical and sublexical reading processes. It was the first to use these optimal stimulus types and show independent fMRI BOLD activation in the ventral- lexical and dorsal-sublexical streams, and further suggested the insular cortex to be sensitive to phonological processing (particularly sublexical spelling-sound translation). |
Campbell, Jamie
Campbell, J. I. (1994). Architectures for numerical cognition. Cognition, 53(1), 1-44. | Different arithmetic notations recruited distinct calculation processes, showing that mental arithmetic is not fundamentally abstract. |
Campbell, J. I., & Xue, Q. (2001). Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(2), 299-315. | Extracurricular cultural activities, rather than differences in formal education, contributed to differences in arithmetic ability between Chinese and North American adults. |
Campbell, J. I., & Graham, D. J. (1985). Mental multiplication skill: Structure, process, and acquisition. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, 39(2), 338-366. | This article introduced the network-interference theory of learning and memory for basic arithmetic facts, which remains an influential model to this day. |
Chirkov, Valery
Cummings, Jorden
Cummings, J. A., Hayes, A. M., Laurenceau, J-P., & Cohen, L. H. (2010). Conflict management mediates the relationship between depressive symptoms and daily negative events: Interpersonal competence and daily stress generation. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 3, 318-331. |
Cummings, J. A., Zagrodney, J. M., & Day, T. E. (2015). Impact of open data policies on consent to participate in human subjects research: Discrepancies between participation action and reported concerns. PLOS ONE, 10(6). |
Cummings, J. A., & Ballantyne, E. C. (2014). What does bad supervision look like? The Behavior Therapist, 37, 230-235. Please contact Dr. Jorden Cummings for more information about this article. |
Desjardins, Michel
Elias, Lorin
Elias, L. J., Bryden, M. P., & Bulman-Fleming, M. B. (1998). Footedness is a better predictor than is handedness of emotional lateralization. Neuropsychologia, 36(1), 37-43. | This paper is widely cited for two reasons. First, because it demonstrates an association between emotional lateralization and foot preference (as opposed to hand preference). Second, it contains a laterality questionnaire I developed, so some of the citations are from people using my measure. |
Saucier, D. M., Green, S. M., Leason, J., MacFadden, A., Bell, S., & Elias, L. J. (2002). Are sex differences in navigation caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences in the ability to use the strategies?. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116(3), 403. | This paper used eye-tracking to look for strategy differences between males and females when learning routes. We found the expected sex differences in the type of navigation, but it wasn't caused by differences in the exploration of the maps. |
Elias, L.J., Saucier, D.M. (2005). Neuropsychology: Clinical and Experimental Foundations. New: York: Pearson/Allyn & Bacon. |
This 200- or 300-level textbook presented a balance of both the experimental and clinical perspectives in neuropsychology, whereas most texts focus mainly on either experimental or clinical content. |
Grant, Peter
Grant, P. R., & Brown, R. (1995). From ethnocentrism to collective protest: Responses to relative deprivation and threats to social identity. Social Psychology Quarterly, 58(3), 195-212. | This paper is widely cited because it is one of the few experimental tests of key hypotheses derived from relative deprivation theory. |
Grant, P.R., & Nadin, S. (2008). The Credentialing Problems of Foreign Trained Personnel from Asia and Africa Intending to Make their Home in Canada: A Social Psychological Perspective. Journal of International Migration and Integration, 8, 141-162. | This study records, in detail, the problems faced by skilled immigrants to Canada from a social psychological perspective using both quantitative and qualitative data. Very few studies use this perspective which is why it is cited frequently. |
Grant, P. R., Abrams, D., Robertson, D. W., & Garay, J. (2015). Predicting Protests by Disadvantaged Skilled Immigrants: A Test of an Integrated Social Identity, Relative Deprivation, Collective Efficacy (SIRDE) Model. Social Justice Research, 28(1), 76-101. |
This very recent paper describes a test of a new theory (the SIRDE model of social change) which integrates social identity theory, relative deprivation theory and resource mobilization theory. The research program that led to the development of this theory started with the paper by Grant & Brown (1995) which is cited above. |
Hunter, Paulette
Hunter, P.V., Antony, M.M. (2009). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of emetophobia: The role of interoceptive exposure. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16(1), 84-91. | At the time I wrote this paper, there was very little published work on the experience of emetophobia. This case study emphasized interoceptive exposure as a potentially useful approach in the treatment of this disorder. Clinical psychology students may also find it useful as an example of one style of case conceptualization. |
Dever Fitzgerald, T., Hunter, P.V., Hadjistavropoulos, T., Koocher, G.P. (2010). Ethical and legal considerations for Internet-based psychotherapy. Cognitive Behavioral Therapy, 39(3), 173-187. | This paper relied on the International Declaration of Ethical Principles for Psychologists as a framework to explore ethical issues that can arise in the context of Internet-based psychotherapy. |
Hunter, P.V., Hadjistavropoulos, T., Smythe, W., Malloy, D., Kaasalainen, S., & Williams, J. (2013). The Personhood in Dementia Questionnaire (PDQ): Establishing an association between beliefs about personhood and health providers’ approaches to person-centred care. Journal of Aging Studies, 27(3), 276–287 |
The three studies that formed my dissertation project are all represented in this paper, which shows that beliefs about the moral rights and psychological and social capacities ("i.e., personhood") of people with advanced dementia influence health providers' intended approaches to care. I continue to be interested in ways to improve the care of people with advanced dementia. |
Lawson, Karen
Lawson, K., Wiggins, S., Green, T., Adam, S., Bloch, M., & Hayden, M. R. (1996). Adverse psychological events occurring in the first year after predictive testing for Huntington's disease. The Canadian Collaborative Study Predictive Testing. Journal of Medical Genetics, 33(10), 856-862. | I published this paper while working as a Research Scientist at the Department of Genetics at UBC. My work there led me to return to university for my PhD to study the intersection between genetic technologies and societal norms. |
Lawson, K. L., & Turriff-Jonasson, S. I. (2006). Maternal serum screening and psychosocial attachment to pregnancy. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60(4), 371-378. | Although there had been much suggestion that maternal serum screening could negatively impact the maternal fetal bond, this work (co-authored with a graduate student) provided the first quantitative evidence that screening may disrupt the developmental trajectory of the maternal-fetal bond, even after favorable results are known. |
Lawson, K. L. (2001). Contemplating selective reproduction: the subjective appraisal of parenting a child with a disability. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 19(1), 73-82. |
The first publication stemming from my PhD dissertation, this paper examines normative attitudes towards persons with disabilities that contribute to the use of prenatal diagnostic testing |
Loehr, Janeen
MacGregor, Michael
McDougall, Patricia
McMullen, Linda
McWilliams, Lachlan
Mickleborough, Marla
Morrison, Melanie
O'Connell, Megan
Mateer, C. A., Sira, C. S., & O'Connell, M. E. (2005). Putting Humpty Dumpty together again: the importance of integrating cognitive and emotional interventions. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 20(1), 62-75. | We call for more integration in rehabilitation for neuropsychological impairments and use case studies to demonstrate efficacy of multifaceted interventions . |
O'Connell, M. E., Mateer, C. A., & Kerns, K. A. (2003). Prosthetic systems for addressing problems with initiation: Guidelines for selection, training, and measuring efficacy. NeuroRehabilitation, 18(1), 9-20. | We review how to incorporate neuropsychological impairments into an intervention plan, We explain how to train people with anterograde amnesia to program and respond to an external cueing device. |
O'Connell, M. E., & Tuokko, H. (2002). The 12-item Buschke memory test: appropriate for use across levels of impairment. Applied Neuropsychology, 9(4), 226-233. |
Olver, Mark
Wormith, J. S., & Olver, M. E. (2002). Offender treatment attrition and its relationship with risk, responsivity, and recidivism. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 29(4), 447-471. | This is an early study examining the prevalence and predictors of attrition from a violent offender treatment program. This study found that actuarially high risk Aboriginal men were particularly vulnerable to attrition, which in turn predicted post-release recidivism. |
Olver, M. E., Wong, S. C., Nicholaichuk, T., & Gordon, A. (2007). The validity and reliability of the Violence Risk Scale-Sexual Offender version: Assessing sex offender risk and evaluating therapeutic change. Psychological Assessment, 19(3), 318-329. | This study examined dynamic sexual violence risk and was one of first to demonstrate linkages between positive treatment change in sexual offenders and decreased recidivism in the community. |
Olver, M. E., Stockdale, K. C., & Wormith, J. S. (2011). A meta-analysis of predictors of offender treatment attrition and its relationship with recidivism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(1), 6-21. | This is a large scale meta-analysis of 114 offender treatment attrition studies across a range of general and specialized programs (domestic violence, sexual offender). A large number of predictors were identified with implications for client retention to minimize attrition. Failure to complete treatment was associated with a 10-23% increase in recidivism, depending on the program and outcome. |
Prime, Steven
Sarty, Gordon
Elias, L. J., Saucier, D. M., Hardie, C., & Sarty, G. E. (2003). Dissociating semantic and perceptual components of synaesthesia: behavioural and functional neuroanatomical investigations. Cognitive Brain Research, 16(2), 232-237. | One of the first fMRI papers on color/number synaesthesia showing real differences in the brain organization of synaesthetes. |
Sarty, G. E., Bennett, R., & Cox, R. W. (2001). Direct reconstruction of non‐Cartesian k‐space data using a nonuniform fast Fourier transform. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 45(5), 908-915. | Mathematical characterization of a widely used image reconstruction technique is MRI. |
Sarty, G. E., Obenaus, A. (2012). Magnetic resonance imaging of astronauts on the international space station and into the solar system. Canadian Aeronautics and Space Journal, 58(1), 60-68. | The beginning of my adventure into putting an MRI into space. |
Sarty, Gordon
Thompson, Valerie
Thompson, V. A. (1994). Interpretational factors in conditional reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 22(6), 742-758. | Relying on beliefs when reasoning logically had long been regarded as an "error" or a "bias". In this paper, I showed that the reliance on beliefs was not an error, but a systematic incorporation of knowledge. Consequently, it is possible to predict, with some degree of accuracy, the types of belief-based inferences people will draw on a logical reasoning task |
Thompson, V. A. (2009). Dual process theories: A metacognitive perspective. In two minds: dual processes and beyond. In J. Evans & K. Frankish. In Two Minds: Dual Processes and Beyond (pp. 171-295). Oxford, UK: Oxford. | Metacognition refers to the processes by which we monitor and control our cognitive processes. In other words, metacognitive processes alert us to when we have made an error, assess confidence in our performance, signal when the current strategy or plan is not working, etc. These processes have been well-studied in the context of memory and education, but never before in the context of reasoning. In these two papers, I introduced a metacognitive theory of reasoning and developed a new paradigm to test that theory. |
Thompson, V.A., Turner, J.A.P., Pennycook, G. (2011). Intuition, reason, and metacognition. Cognitive Psychology, 63(3), 107-140. |
Wormith, J. Stephen
Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., & Wormith, J. S. (2006). The recent past and near future of risk and/or need assessment. Crime and Delinquency, 52(1), 7. | This review discussed risk assessment in the context of risk, need and responsivity. It challenged traditional mental heath approaches to risk assessment and responded to feminist critiques. |
Wormith, J. S., & Olver, M. E. (2002). Offender treatment attrition and its relationship with risk, responsivity, and recidivism. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 29(4), 447-471. | We examine patterns and predictors of offender attrition from a high intensity violent offender treatment program at the RPC. |
Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., & Wormith, J. S. (2011). THE RISK-NEED-RESPONSIVITY (RNR) MODEL Does Adding the Good Lives Model Contribute to Effective Crime Prevention? Criminal Justice and Behavior, 38(7), 735-755. |